DATE: July 30, 2019
TO: Josh Brammer, Office of Information and Regulatory Affairs, Office of Management and Budget
FROM: Christine Fortunato, Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families
RE: Request for non-substantive change to the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-being Third Cohort (NSCAW III): Data Collection (Phase II)
This memorandum requests approval for a non-substantive change to the information collection (IC) effort approved under OMB Control Number 0970-0202. The purpose of this request is to make the following changes to the 18-month follow-up data collection (Wave 2):
Expand the child age range for the Go/No-Go assessment1 of executive functioning, and
Remove the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task.
Information about child executive functioning is not a new topic for this IC. The minor change requested would expand the age range of children administered the assessment from the age of 5 or older to 4 or older. Further, due to feasibility issues around implementation and costs, we are unable to administer the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task as planned.
Since this IC was approved in July 2017, additional resources on the assessment of executive function in young children are publicly available. Specifically, the Executive Function Mapping Project Measures Compendium (Bailey et al., 2018) provides a summary of available executive function assessments, appropriate age ranges, and research studies that have implemented the assessments. A review of Compendium indicated that the Go/No-Go assessment planned for Wave 2 could be administered to children as young as 3 years of age. Further, consultation with experts confirmed successful administration of the Go/No-Go assessment with children as young as 3 or 4-years old. Because 3-year-old children participating in NSCAW receive multiple, complex direct child assessments that require sustained attention and longer interview administration times, we have determined that starting at 4 years old for the Go/No-Go assessment would be most appropriate. See Attachment 1 for a full list of literature reviewed in support of this request.
Overall, collecting executive function assessments from young children is intended to support a better understanding of attentional control and executive functioning in a vulnerable cohort of young children. Information on executive function is not currently available in national data collection efforts focused on child maltreatment and foster care placements and adoptions.
We expect the requested change to be advantageous in that it will allow for the examination of attentional control and executive function in a wider age range of children. Because NSCAW is a longitudinal study, child performance on this assessment could be examined as a predictor of longer-term outcomes, including academic achievement, school engagement, and delinquent behaviors. Collecting this information as part of this ongoing effort is highly relevant given the lack of nationally representative data on young child executive function, particularly in a vulnerable population.
Summary of Proposed Changes
The proposed change is restricted to the “Executive Functioning” (EF) module in the follow-up Child Interview. To expand the age range of children administered from 5 or older to 4 or older, we request the following revisions (additions and updates shown in red).
IF
CHILD AGE IS
4 OR
OLDER, administer
the two
tests
of executive functioning: the Shape Go-NoGo Task and
then the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task
The Shape Go-NoGo task consists of a series of single shapes that “flash” on the laptop screen. The child is instructed to press a button on the laptop as quickly as possible for all shapes except a circle; the child tries not to press the key anytime the circle appears.
In
the Dimensional Change Card Sort task, the child will be shown
pictures on the laptop that vary along two dimensions (e.g., colored
shapes such as yellow balls and blue trucks). The child will be
asked to first match pictures according to one dimension (e.g.,
color), and then according to the other dimension (shape).
Attachment 1: Literature Reviewed in Support of this Request
Archibald, S. J., & Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and description of new tests of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychology, 5(2), 115-129.
Bailey, R., Barnes, S. P., Park, C., Sokolovic, N., & Jones, S. M. (2018). Executive Function Mapping Project Measures Compendium: A Resource for Selecting Measures Related to Executive Function and Other Regulation-related Skills in Early Childhood. OPRE Report # 2018-59, . Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Karalunas, S. L., Bierman, K. L., & Huang-Pollock, C. L. (2016). Test–retest reliability and measurement invariance of executive function tasks in young children with and without ADHD. Journal of attention disorders, 1087054715627488.
Kuntsi, J., Andreou, P., Ma, J., Börger, N. A., & van der Meere, J. J. (2005). Testing assumptions for endophenotype studies in ADHD: reliability and validity of tasks in a general population sample. BMC psychiatry, 5(1), 40.
Langenecker, S. A., Zubieta, J.-K., Young, E. A., Akil, H., & Nielson, K. A. (2007). A task to manipulate attentional load, set-shifting, and inhibitory control: Convergent validity and test–retest reliability of the Parametric Go/No-Go Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(8), 842-853.
Simmonds, D. J., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2008). Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 224-232.
Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R., & Greenberg, M. (2010). The measurement of executive function at age 3 years: psychometric properties and criterion validity of a new battery of tasks. Psychological assessment, 22(2), 306.
Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R., & Blair, C. B. (2012). Executive function in early childhood: Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological assessment, 24(2), 418.
Winter, W., & Sheridan, M. (2014). Previous reward decreases errors of commission on later ‘No‐Go’trials in children 4 to 12 years of age: evidence for a context monitoring account. Developmental science, 17(5), 797-807.
Wright, L., Lipszyc, J., Dupuis, A., Thayapararajah, S. W., & Schachar, R. (2014). Response inhibition and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of go/no-go task performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 429.
Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self‐regulation. Child development perspectives, 6(2), 112-121.
1 In the Go/No-Go assessment, a child is asked to press a button whenever a target stimulus (e.g., circle) is shown, but to inhibit this response when a non-target stimulus (e.g., square) is shown. Response time and accuracy (commission and omission errors) are measured.
File Type | application/vnd.openxmlformats-officedocument.wordprocessingml.document |
Author | admin |
File Modified | 0000-00-00 |
File Created | 2022-05-19 |