Appendix 2 - FES Experimental Testing 2013-0926

0648-0652 Appendix 2 - FES Experimental Testing 2013-0926.pdf

Marine Recreational Information Program Fishing Effort Survey

Appendix 2 - FES Experimental Testing 2013-0926

OMB: 0648-0652

Document [pdf]
Download: pdf | pdf
Marine Recreational Information Program
Fishing Effort Survey
Experimental Testing
9/26/2013

The MRIP Fishing Effort Survey (MFES) was implemented in Massachusetts, New York, North
Carolina and Florida in October, 2012 to test a revised data collection design for monitoring
marine recreational fishing effort. The survey, which collects information for two-month
reference waves, included two experiments during the first two study waves, wave 5 (Sept-Oct
2012) and wave 6 (Nov-Dec, 2012), to test different survey design features aimed at maximizing
efficiency and minimizing nonresponse error. Specifically, the experiments tested two versions
of the survey instrument and four levels of cash incentives. Details of the experiments are
provided below.
Instrument Testing
The MFES included an experiment to test two versions of the survey instrument. The objective
of the experiment was to identify the instrument that maximized overall response rates while
minimizing the potential for nonresponse bias resulting from differential nonresponse between
anglers and non-anglers. One version of the instrument (Saltwater Fishing Survey) utilized a
“screen out” approach that quickly identifies anglers (and non-anglers) and encourages
participation by minimizing the number of survey questions, particularly for non-anglers.
Person-level information, including details about recent fishing activity and limited demographic
information, is collected for all household residents, but only if someone in the household
reported fishing during the reference wave. The second version (Weather and Outdoor Activity
Survey) utilized an “engaging” approach that encourages response by broadening the scope of
the questions to include both fishing and non-fishing questions. This version collects personlevel information for all residents of sampled households, regardless of whether or not household
residents participated in saltwater fishing. Each wave, sampled addresses were randomly
assigned to one of the two questionnaire types, which were evaluated in terms of response rates
and reported fishing activity.
Table 1 provides the weighted response rates (AAPOR RR1 after excluding undeliverable
addresses) and estimated fishing prevalence (percentage of households with residents who
reported fishing during the wave) for the two versions of the instrument. Overall, the Weather
and Outdoor Activity Survey achieved a significantly higher response rate than the Saltwater
Fishing Survey, and there was no significant difference between instruments in estimated
prevalence. The lack of a significant difference between instruments for estimated prevalence
suggests that the gain in response for the engaging instrument cannot be attributed to increased
survey participation by either anglers or non-anglers, but that both groups are more likely to
respond to the Weather and Outdoor Activity Survey than the Saltwater Fishing Survey.
We also compared response rates and prevalence between instruments both among and within
subpopulations defined by whether or not sampled addresses could be matched to state databases
of licensed saltwater anglers – subpopulations expected to distinguish between households with
anglers and households with no anglers or less avid anglers. As expected, both response rates
and estimated prevalence were higher in the matched subpopulation than the unmatched
subpopulation, confirming that a population expected to be interested in the survey topic households with licensed anglers - is more likely to respond to a fishing survey and report fishing

activity than a population that excludes licensed anglers 1. Because we can identify household
license status prior to data collection, we can account for differential nonresponse between
matched and unmatched households in the estimation design by treating matched an unmatched
domains as strata (Lohr, 2009).
Table 1. Weighted response rates and estimated prevalence overall and by domain for two
versions of the survey instrument.

Saltwater Fishing
Survey
(%)
(n)

Weather and Outdoor
Activity Survey
(%)
(n)

Response Rate
Overall
Matched
Unmatched

31.1 (0.4)
45.4 (1.1)
30.3 (0.4)

17,511
3,160
14,351

34.7 (0.4)*
45.0 (1.0)
34.0 (0.5)*

17,510
3,247
14,263

Prevalence
Overall
Matched
Unmatched

13.4 (0.5)
49.9 (1.7)
11.2 (0.6)

5,943
1,491
4,452

14.1 (0.5)
48.5 (1.6)
12.2 (0.6)

6,498
1,552
4,946

Notes – (1) standard errors are in parentheses. (2) Domains are defined by matching ABS
samples to state databases of licensed saltwater anglers.
*Significantly different from Saltwater Fishing Survey (p<0.05).
There were no significant differences between instruments for either response rate or prevalence
within the matched domain, suggesting that the inclusion of non-fishing questions in the Weather
and Outdoor Activity Survey did not have an impact on response by either anglers or nonanglers. In the unmatched domain, the response rate was significantly higher for the Weather
and Outdoor Activity Survey than the Saltwater Fishing Survey. However, the higher response
rate did not translate to lower or higher estimates of prevalence; estimates of prevalence were not
significantly different between instruments within the domain. This suggests that the engaging
instrument uniformly increased the probability of response for anglers and non-anglers within the
unmatched domain.
Differential nonresponse to a survey request between subpopulations will result in nonresponse
bias if the subpopulations are different with respect to the survey topic. In the MRIP Fishing
Effort Survey, we account for differential nonresponse between matched and unmatched
households during sampling – matched and unmatched subpopulations are treated as independent
The classification of sample into domains is dependent upon matching ABS sample to license databases by
address and telephone number. This process is unlikely to be 100% accurate, so the unmatched domain is likely to
include some households with licensed anglers. The unmatched domain also includes households with residents
who fish without a license.

1

strata. Subsequently, the potential for nonresponse bias is limited to differential nonresponse
between anglers and non-anglers within the matched and unmatched subpopulations. While the
Weather and Outdoor Activity Survey achieved a higher response rate than the Saltwater Fishing
Survey, both overall and within the unmatched subpopulation, the gains in response do not
appear to result from a higher propensity to respond to the survey by either anglers or nonanglers. As a result, we cannot conclude that one of the instruments is more or less likely to
minimize differential nonresponse between anglers and non-anglers. However, higher response
rates decrease the risk for nonresponse bias and either lower data collection costs (for a fixed
sample size) or increase the precision of estimates (for a fixed cost) 2. Consequently, we
conclude that the Weather and Outdoor Activity Survey is superior to the Saltwater Fishing
Survey and recommend that the instrument be utilized for subsequent survey waves. Because it
collects person-level information for all residents of all sampled households, the Weather and
Outdoor Activity Survey also supports post-stratification of survey weights to population
controls, which is an additional benefit of this recommendation.
Incentive Testing
The MRIP Fishing Effort Survey included an experiment to test the impact of modest, prepaid
cash incentives on survey response and survey measures. Each wave, sampled addresses were
randomly allocated to incentive treatment groups of $1, $2, and $5, as well as a non-incentive
control group. Incentives were only included in the initial survey mailing. As in the instrument
experiment, the objective of the incentive testing was to identify an optimum level of incentive
that maximizes overall response while controlling costs and minimizes the potential for
nonresponse bias resulting from differential nonresponse between anglers and non-anglers.
Response rates, estimated fishing prevalence and relative costs of completing an interview were
compared among incentive treatments to quantify the impacts of incentives.
Table 2 shows weighted response rates and the results of a logistic regression model predicting
the effects of incentives on the odds of obtaining a completed survey. Including an incentive in
the initial survey mailing significantly increased the odds of receiving a completed survey, and
the odds increased significantly as the incentive amount increased. Cash incentives of $1, $2,
and $5 increased the odds of receiving a completed survey by 63%, 93% and 137%, respectively.
Table 2. Weighted response rates and odds of receiving a completed survey by incentive
amount.

Incentive
$0
$1
$2
$5

Response
Rate (%)
22.6
32.2
36
40.8

n
8,760
8,737
8,738
8,786

Odds Ratio
1.00
1.63*
1.93*
2.37*

95 % CI
(1.51, 1.77)
(1.78, 2.09)
(2.18, 2.56)

*Significantly different from the $0 control (p<0.05). Results of pairwise comparisons are as
follows: $1>$0 (p<0.05), $2>$1 (p<0.05), $5>$2 (p<0.05).
2

Assuming that fixed costs are the same for the two instruments, which was the case in the experiment.

Previous studies (Groves et al., 2006) have demonstrated that prepaid cash incentives can
motivate individuals with little or no interest in a survey topic to respond to a survey request.
Subsequently, we hypothesized that incentives would have a larger impact on non-anglers than
anglers, minimizing differential nonresponse between the two populations. We initially explored
this hypothesis by comparing estimated fishing prevalence among incentive conditions,
expecting that gains in response in the incentive conditions would translate to lower estimates of
fishing prevalence. The results do not support this hypothesis; there were no significant
differences in prevalence among incentive conditions (Table 3).
Table 3. Overall estimated fishing prevalence by incentive amount.

Incentive
$0
$1
$2
$5

Prevalence
(%)
12.8
14.1
13.6
14.1

n
2,154
3,065
3,415
3,807

Note – Differences in prevalence among treatments are not significant (p=0.05)
We further explored the interaction of topic salience and incentives by examining response rates
and estimated fishing prevalence for the incentive conditions within domains defined by whether
or not sampled addresses could be matched to databases of licensed saltwater anglers. We
expected incentives to have a more pronounced effect in the unmatched domain, a population
less likely to have an interest in the survey topic, than in the matched domain. Table 4 shows
that incentives increased the odds of receiving a completed survey in both the matched and
unmatched subpopulations. However, the value of the incentive seems to be more important in
the unmatched domain, where the odds of receiving a completed survey increased uniformly and
significantly as the value of the incentive increased ($0<$1<$2<$5). In contrast, the incentive
amount was less significant in the matched domain, where the odds of receiving a completed
survey were relatively flat among incentive conditions. These results are consistent with our
expectations and suggest that a population with a low propensity to respond to a fishing survey
can be motivated to participate by cash incentives, and that the motivation may increase as the
incentive amount increases.

Table 4. Odds of receiving a completed survey by level of incentive for sample that could and
could not be matched to state databases of licensed anglers.

Comparison
Pair
$1 vs. $0
$2 vs. $0
$5 vs. $0
$2 vs. $1
$5 vs. $1
$5 vs. $2

Subpopulation
Matched
Unmatched
OR
OR
1.75**
1.63**
2.01**
1.93**
2.11**
2.39**
1.15
1.18**
1.21*
1.46**
1.05
1.24**

Notes – The second value in the comparison pair is the reference value.
Significance: *p<0.05, **p<0.0001
As noted previously, we expected that the gains in response in the incentive conditions would
translate to lower estimates of fishing prevalence, particularly in the unmatched subpopulation.
Once again, the results are not consistent with expectations; differences in fishing prevalence
among treatments were not significant in either the matched or unmatched domain (Table 5).
The lack of an effect of incentives on fishing prevalence suggests that the gains in response
associated with increasing incentive amounts are uniform between anglers and non-anglers.
Table 5. Estimated fishing prevalence by incentive amount for a population of anglers (matched)
and non-anglers (unmatched).

Incentive
$0
$1
$2
$5

Subpopulation
Matched
Unmatched
(%)
(n)
(%)
(n)
49.2
533
10.7
1,621
50.3
779
12
2,286
48.6
837
11.6
2,578
48.2
894
12.4
2,913

Note – Within subpopulations differences in prevalence among treatments are not significant
(p=0.05)

We also examined the effect of cash incentives on overall data collection costs, specifically the
direct costs of printing, postage, and the cash incentives themselves. Table 6 shows that the $5
incentive provided the largest gain in response, but the gain came at a relative cost of
approximately $0.15 per completed interview. In contrast, the additional costs of the $1 and $2
incentives (20% and 38% higher cost than the $0 control, respectively) are more than offset by
the associated gains in the number of completed surveys (42% and 58%, respectively). In other
words, including a $1 or $2 cash incentive in the initial survey mailing actually decreased the
cost of receiving a completed survey by 22% and 20%, respectively. These cost savings, which

are conservative 3, could be used to lower overall data collection costs (for a fixed sample size) or
increase the precision of survey estimates (for a fixed cost).
Table 6. Effect of incentives on data collection costs
Incentive
Amount
$0
$1
$2
$5

Relative Cost
Difference
1.00
1.20
1.38
1.90

Relative Difference
in Completed
Surveys
1
1.42
1.58
1.75

Relative Cost
per Completed
Survey
$1.00
$0.78
$0.80
$1.15

Note – relative differences reflect the ratio of quantities (cost, completes) in the experimental
treatments to the zero dollar control.
Including a modest prepaid cash incentive in survey mailings clearly has a positive effect on
survey response rates; the odds of receiving a completed survey increased significantly as the
incentive amount increased. We expected the incentives to have a greater effect on non-anglers
than anglers and decrease the potential for nonresponse bias by minimizing differential
nonresponse between these two populations. However, the results of the experiment suggest that
incentives increase response propensities for non-anglers and anglers equally. While this result
does not support our hypothesis, it does demonstrate that incentives can increase the quantity of
data without having a negative impact on survey measures. The experiment also demonstrated
that incentives can decrease overall data collection costs. Based upon these findings, we
conclude that a $2 incentive is optimal in terms of both maximizing response rates and
minimizing data collection costs.

The cost comparison assumes that the non-incentive direct costs (postage and printing) are the same for all
survey treatments and does not reflect the fact that incentive conditions may not require as many follow-up
mailings.

3

References
Groves, R.M., M.P. Couper, S. Presser, E. Singer, R. Tourangeau, G.P. Piani, N. Lindsay. 2006.
Experiments in producing nonresponse bias. Public Opinion Quarterly, 70: 720-736.
Lohr, S. 2009. Multiple Frame Surveys. Chapter 4 in Pfeffermann, D. (Ed.) Handbook of
Statistics: Sample Surveys Design, Methods and Applications (vol. 29A). Elsevier,
Amsterdam.


File Typeapplication/pdf
AuthorRob_Andrews
File Modified2019-12-06
File Created2019-09-17

© 2024 OMB.report | Privacy Policy